Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area
Volume 29.1 — April 2006
COMPOUNDING AND THE STRUCTURE
OF THE TANI LEXICON1
Mark Post
Research Centre for Linguistic Typology
La Trobe University
Abstract to go here
Keywords:
1. INTRODUCTION
Proto-Tani (Tibeto-Burman > Tani), as partially reconstructed by Sun (1993) and continued by
Post (in progress), shows signs of having been, or having had a near ancestor which was, a
“morphosyllabic” language more or less according to the Mainland Southeast Asian prototype.
However, the modern Tani languages are all synthetic and agglutinating, to varying degrees. One
aspect of this overall shift in morphological typology appears to have been a progressive
restructuring of the lexicon, in which a basic monosyllabism shifted to a basic disyllabism as what
were previously productively formed collocations (compounds and prefixed roots) were
lexicalised. In the process, a division emerged in at least some Tani languages in which nouns
and adjectives were closely aligned in opposition to verbs. The nature of this division is the
subject of the present paper.
In §2, we review the genetic and geographical context of the Tani languages. This is followed
by some more background concerning the nature of “morphosyllabic” typology in §3; §3 also
reviews some of the arguments in favour of recognising a typological shift in Tani from
morphosyllabism to greater synthesis and agglutination. Section §4 describes the evolution and
present state of the Tani lexicon, particularly as exemplified by the Lare dialect of Galo (Tani >
Western > Galo) and we conclude with some further speculations in §5.
2. GENETIC AND GEOGRAPHICAL CONTEXT OF THE TANI LANGUAGES
Following Sun’s (1993) landmark study, it is now possible to identify the Tani languages
(formerly known as “Abor-Miri-Dafla” or “Mirish”) as a distinct branch of Tibeto-Burman (TB).
Its closest affinities are with the Digarish (Taraon-Idu Mishmi) languages to the east/southeast of
the Tani area; affinities also exist to Hrusish and Mizu Mishmi (Kaman), although the possibility
of higher-order groupings has not yet been extensively researched. Internally, the primary
division in Tani is between an Eastern and a Western branch. Figure 1 reproduces Sun’s (1993)
provisional Tani stammbaum (slightly adjusted), which is constructed on the basis of shared
innovations and retentions in the segmental phonology as well as lexical isoglosses.
Eastern Tani languages are, in general, more conservative than Western Tani in segmental
phonology, and are also relatively more synthetic and agglutinating. Western Tani languages
have undergone numerous coda-erosions and onset simplifications and mergers, and in some ways
retain a less synthetic morphological profile. The lower-level subgrouping is more difficult, both
1
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 15th Annual Meeting of the Southeast Asian Linguistic
Society, April 21-23, 2005, at the Australian National University (Canberra).
1
2
Mark Post
since we continue to lack reliable data on any scale for most Tani languages, and since a number
of innovations are shared among members of different branches.2
Sino-Tibetan > Tibeto-Burman > Tani
Western
Eastern
Apatani
Milang?
Bokar?
Damu?
Nyisu, Bengni, Nyishi, Tagin,
Yano, Hills Miri, Galo?
Bori
Mising, Paadam,
Minyong, Pailibo?
Figure 1. Provisional Tani subgroupings (based on Sun (1993:297); sister branchings are
replaced by ‘uncertainty’ triangles; Minyong and Pailibo added)
The vast majority of the Tani languages are spoken in mountainous central Arunachal
Pradesh, Northeast India, although the largest single population of Tani language speakers is the
plains-dwelling Mising of Upper Assam. Small numbers of Tani language speakers are also
found in Tibet, which is almost certainly the Tani place of origin (Blackburn 2003/2004) (Figure
2).
Tibetan Memba
Monpa
Hruso
Idu
Digaru
Mizu
Tai
Assamese
Boro
Tani
language
area
Figure 2. The geographical and language contact situation of the Tani languages
There has been an enormous increase in the exposure of Tani speakers to Indo-European
languages such as Hindi, Assamese, Nepali and English over the last 40-50 years, and numerous
lexical items, grammatical morphemes and grammatical constructions have been borrowed from
these languages, particularly into more southerly Tani languages such as Galo, Mising and Nyishi
and in the speech of (usually multi-lingual) younger and well-educated speakers. However,
2 Probably, this complexity is reflective of the ongoing (mainly southward) migration of Tani peoples and their
consequent shifts into and out of contact with one another.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
3
relatively little evidence of any effects of Indic language contact prior to the immediate past has
been found, and it is doubtful that Indic language contact alone can account for the shift in
morphological typology that seems to have occurred in Tani.
3. TYPOLOGICAL DRIFT IN TANI
3.1. Morphosyllabic typology
The useful term “morphosyllabic” was coined by Timothy Light in the context of tonogenesis
theory (Light 1978), although it has regrettably gained little currency since. Found commonly in
Mainland South East Asia, a prototypical morphosyllabic language exhibits a fundamental unity
in which syllable = morpheme = word. Very little well-grammaticalised material is found, and
affixal morphology in general is often lacking; instead, grammatical functions are most often
coded by variants of lexical words. Accordingly, there is a strongly analytic, isolating grammar,
with verb serialisation usually featuring prominently. Frequently, and perhaps typically, lexical
tones are also found, and usually relate to a simple or simplifying segment inventory and
canonical syllable structure. (1) briefly exemplifies these features in a fairly prototypical
morphosyllabic language, Standard Thai. In the example, each word is monomorphemic and each
morpheme is realised by a tone-bearing, monosyllabic, segmentally simple word; serialisation is
the main grammatical strategy employed.
(1) T (khǎw)…kɔ̂
piin
klàp
khɨ ̂n
paj
kèp
tɔɔ
(3)
SFOC climb return ascend go
collect continue
‘So, (he) climbed back up and continued picking (pears).’ (Post in press-a:7)3
3.2. Proto-Tani as a probable morphosyllabic language
There is a large body of evidence to support the notion that Proto-Tani (or Pre-Proto-Tani) was
basically a morphosyllabic language, although we won’t have space here to review everything in
detail (more detailed arguments are to be found in Post (in progress)). However, we can note the
following facts.
1) Basically monosyllabic lexicon. Almost all of the forms reconstructed by Sun (1993)
represent monosyllabic roots. While roots are usually bound in modern Tani languages (either in
the sense of being compound elements or in the sense of requiring an affix to occur as a
grammatical word), there is evidence that Proto-Tani roots were potentially free. For example, in
certain modern Tani languages such as Apatani, verb roots occur unaffixed as predicates with an
inherent Perfective aspect (2). Since it is rare, in general, for languages to simply discontinue the
use of affixes without leaving so much as a trace, it seems more likely that predication by simple
roots represents a conservation, rather than an innovation, in Apatani.
3 All data herein are from field texts, field elicitations, or published sources where noted, with the language codes
T=Thai, G=Galo, M=Mising and A=Apatani given to the right of the numeral. Other language codes used include
PTS=Proto-Tani Sun (1993) and PG=Proto-Galo. Tani language transcriptions have been regularised to follow IPA
except where c = [tɕ] and z = [dʑ]. Thai transcription follows IPA except where kh = [kʰ]. Note that in Tani
transcription (unless otherwise noted) unmarked syllables are underlyingly toneless and are tonally specified via
spreading rules, whereas in Thai transcription, unmarked syllables are almost always specified for mid tone, which
carries a definite contour. I thank my Thai consultant, Duangkamol Sutthiwari, my Galo consultants Igo Riba, Tomo
Riba and Mili Nyodu, and my Mising consultant Ain Doley.
4
Mark Post
(2) A ŋo
lu
ŋo
iŋ
1.SG say.PFV
1.SG go.PFV
‘I spoke.’
‘I went.’
(Abraham 1985:93, glosses adjusted by this author. Note that syllable tones are present in
Apatani, but are not consistently marked in the source)
In addition, polysyllabic lexemes and grammatical morphemes are almost always easily
analysable as earlier collocations of monosyllabic morphemes, and while the patterns underlying
their composition are generally unproductive in modern Tani languages, cross-language variation
suggests proto-compositionality, productivity and variability. Consider e.g. ‘road’ (PT *lam),
with the modern Mising, Apatani, and Lare Galo forms lambə, lenda, and bədáa respectively
(with the cognate-root patterns A-B, A-C and B-C in order). Or compare the Paglo Mising
postposition tolokə ‘from eastward’ with Galo tə̀ ‘Demonstrative (upward)’, Apatani lo
‘Instrumental postposition’ and Apatani kɨ ‘Genitive postposition’.4 Numerous examples such as
these may be found.
2) Easily reconstructible morphology. Modern Tani languages exhibit a dizzying array of
grammatical morphemes, most of them suffixes. However, the majority are easily reconstructible
to lexical roots which either continue to occur in the same language or can be found in another
Tani language. For example, Lare Galo Applicative suffix -rɨ ́k has no lexical counterpart in Galo,
but is easily relatable to the Paglo Mising verb root rɨk- ‘meet’. Furthermore, unpredictable sound
changes potentially associated with grammaticalisation (such as erosion) are found only rarely.
Both of these facts suggest relatively recent development from lexical roots.
3) Lack of irregularities. There are few if any morphophonological alternations, inflection
classes or paradigmatic irregularities in modern Tani languages that could serve to support
reconstruction of earlier affixal morphology.
4) Lexical tone. Although a firm statement on the reconstruction of lexical tones to Proto-Tani
awaits adequate documentation of tones in an Eastern Tani language, comparative Western Tani
data analysed to-date supports reconstruction of tones at least to Proto-Western-Tani (Post 2005).
However, segmental correspondences are not suggestive of tonogenesis at that stage, indicating
that tones may well be reconstructible to Proto-Tani. Within Western Tani, despite the often large
number of syllables that may make up a phonological word, the underlying tone bearing unit is
always a monosyllabic lexical root or grammatical morpheme which is reconstructible to an
earlier lexical root.
5) Simple segmental phonology/syllable structure. In contrast with some nearby TB languages
such as Sherdukpen or Mizu Mishmi, Tani languages have a simple segmental phonology and
core syllable canon. Sun (1993) reconstructs voiced and voiceless stops and voiced nasals (labial,
coronal, palatal and velar), in addition to voiced and voiceless fricatives (labial, coronal, palatal
and glottal), three approximants, and seven vowels. The basic syllable canon (Ci)(G)V1(X)[T?]5
is preserved by most modern Tani languages, although many have lost the medial approximants *j- and *-r- (and *-w-, if it existed), most have lost or merged a number of proto-onsets and proto4 The ‘east’ ↔ ‘up’ correspondence is explained by the fact that the pan-Tani deictic frame, which operates on
motion verbs as well as locational postpositions and demonstratives, experienced a shift in Mising from ‘up’ -‘down’ to ‘east’ -- ‘west’. This correspondence probably has nothing to do with the rising and setting sun, but relates
instead to the fact that the Mising historically migrated from the uplands, where the system evolved, to the plains,
where no mountains (or even many small hills) are present – but where the Brahmaputra river, the overwhelmingly
dominant feature of life on the plains, flows from east to west.
5
Ci = initial consonant, Cf = final consonant, G = medial approximants /r/, /j/ and possibly also /w/, X = V1 or Cf,
and [T?] = possible tone.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
5
rhymes, a few west-central languages such as Pugo Galo, Hills Miri and possibly Nyishi have, due
to lexicalisation of a widespread process of word-final short vowel deletion in that area, innovated
both voiced coda consonants and a previously nonexistent syllable type with the structure
(C)VVC, and some (such as at least some varieties of Mising) have apparently lost tones, if
indeed tones occurred at the Proto-Tani stage.
In sum, the morphological and phonological facts both support a view of Proto-Tani as a
likely morphosyllabic language.
3.3. Shift to greater synthesis/agglutination
As already noted in passing, most modern Tani languages are characterised by a predominance of
bound rather than free forms and a wide array of grammatical suffixes and enclitics. In the
following Paglo Mising sentence, the string of morphemes forming the predicate is a single word
according to phonological and grammatical criteria.
(3) M ŋo
ŋo
gɨladakkubon?
gɨ-la-dak-ku-bo-n
1.SG go-ABIL-IMMD-CMPL-IRR-PQ
‘Can I go now (having finished the work)?’
In addition to the morphological facts, we can also note that data from historical sound
changes suggest an increase in sensitivity to the word rather than the syllable as the basic prosodic
unit. For example, in a probably related series of early Western Tani Palatalisation changes,
bilabial and velar onsets palatalised before front vowels and glides (e.g. PTS *bi > Lare Galo zí‘give’, and PTS *ken > Lare Galo cèn- ‘know’); these changes affected every qualifying syllable
in a language which underwent them. By contrast, in many later changes, such as Final Vowel
Weakening in Lare Galo (in which word-final PG *-a and *-ɨ reduce to -ə), the domain was the
word rather than the syllable (e.g. PG *tabɨ ́ > Lare Galo tabə́ ‘snake’, but cf. Lare Galo bɨrə́m
‘python’ in which the word-initial root retains the proto-vocalism). Thus, while early sound
changes introduced opacity between languages, they preserved the phonological integrity of
monosyllabic roots within a given language. However, later changes began to introduce opacity
among instantiations of the same root within a given language, which reflected an increase in the
basic status of complex polysyllables over simplex monosyllables in the Tani lexicon.6
4. EVOLUTION OF THE TANI LEXICON
4.1. Roots versus words
Tani roots are almost always monosyllabic. In §3.2, we argued that monosyllabic roots probably
patterned as independent words and/or in productively-formed collocations at the Proto-Tani
stage. However, in most modern Tani languages roots are bound, and cannot in general occur as
underived lexemes.
6 Many of the word-level sound changes we find seem to be motivated by or at least related to the occurrence of a
trochaic metrical foot in at least some of the languages in which they are observed (e.g., in my data, the Lare and
Pugo dialects of Galo, Paglo Mising, Pasigat Minyong and Hills Miri). It is very possible that the rise of a wordstress system in Tani languages in itself explains many of the phenomena we are here associating somewhat
diagnostically to a shift in morphological typology (see e.g. Donegan and Stampe (1983; 2004) for related
discussion). However, in absence of almost any data at all relating to the prosodic systems of other Tani languages,
we cannot at present do more than speculate about the nature of Proto-Tani prosody and developments since then.
6
Mark Post
4.2. Major lexical classes
In many modern Tani languages, the major lexical classes found are noun, adjective and verb.
They may differ from language to language in their alignment and/or the extent to which they are
distinct,7 however in Lare Galo we can identify a clear three-way distinction on the basis of
behaviour in predicate environments: nominals are supported by an uninflecting copula, but
cannot take aspect-marking (4); verbs take aspect-marking, but cannot occur with a copula (5);
adjectives are predicated under either condition, with a minor semantic difference (6). Figure 3
schematises this arrangement.
(4) G əgə̀
ikiə̀
*əgə̀
ikîidu
əgə̀
ikìi=əə
əgə̀
ikìi-dùu
DST.IND
dog=COP.IPFV
DST.IND
dog-IPFV
‘it’s a dog’
(5) G əgə̀
jubdù
*əgə̀
jubə̀
əgə̀
jùp-dùu
əgə̀
jùp=əə
DST.IND
sleep-IPFV
DST.IND
sleep=COP.IPFV
‘it’s sleeping’
(6) G əgə̀
əgə̀
adəkə́
əgə̀
adəkdù
adə́k=əə
əgə
adə́k-dùu
DST.IND
different=COP.IPFV
‘it’s different (appraising a present
state-of-affairs)’
Tests:
DST.IND
different-IPFV
‘it’s different (now, and in
general)’
Copular Predication
Nouns
Aspect-Marking
Adjectives
Verbs
Figure 3. Tests for identifying major word classes in Galo
Non-derived adverbs are not well-attested in Tani. Most commonly adverbial concepts are
coded either by derivational verb suffixes (7), or by adverbials derived from adjectives (8).
However, a small number of time nouns have adverbial functions in at least some Tani languages,
and could well be described as an emergent adverb class (9).
7 Sun (2003), for example, claims that adjectives are a subclass of verb throughout Tani on the basis that in some
Tani languages (mainly of the Western branch) adjectives are, like verbs, predicated via uninflecting auxiliaries.
However, later examples suggest that the same uninflecting auxiliary also supports a nominal predication (Sun 2003:
ex.1, 9, and 22), leaving uncertainty as to what, in fact, distinguishes any one word class from any other. While it is
certainly possible (and indeed likely) that at least some Tani languages either retain or have renovated a Protoalignment of verb-adjective in opposition to nouns in predicate environments (at least), the case has yet to be
convincingly made.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
(7) G ŋunù
tolò
inmênrə
ŋunù
tolò
ín-mèn-rə́
7
1.PL LOC.UPgo-AS.PLAY-IRR
‘We’ll go for a walk (to) up there.’ (lit. ‘We’ll go playfully (to) up there’)
(8) G aləbə́
intòla
dêi!
alə́=bə́
ín-tó-làa
dêi
good=AVZR
‘Go safely!’
good-IPTV.ODIR-NF
HORT
(9) G ŋunù
ŋunù
hilò
tolûu
hilòo tolûu
inmênrə!
ín-mèn-rə́
1.PL today LOC.UP.EXT go-AS.PLAY-IRR
‘Today we’ll go for a walk (to) waaaay up there!’
The following sections describe the evolution of the major lexical classes in more detail. The
data are mainly from Lare Galo; however, similar structures are found in most other Tani
languages for which adequate data exists.
4.3. Evolution of the nominal/adjectival lexicon
Most Tani languages – particularly those of the Western branch – preserve at least a few simplex
monosyllabic nouns and adjectives, such as Apatani si ‘urine’ and khɨ ‘six(th)’ (Abraham 1987:
99, 30) and Bokar loo ‘day’ and hum ‘three’ (Ouyang 1985: 58, cited in Sun (2003)). However,
the overwhelming majority of nouns and adjectives in all Tani languages are disyllabic, with the
relatively small number of monosyllables usually analysable as etymologically complex. In Lare
Galo, only two undoubtedly simplex monosyllabic lexemes have been found to occur in a lexicon
of almost 3000 words recorded to date: ɲíi ‘human being’ and zèe ‘grue (green-blue)’. In Paglo
Mising, no simplex lexemes have yet been found.
Disyllabic nouns and adjectives are always etymologically complex, with the following types
of pattern underlying their composition:
1. prefixed root
2. root-root compound
3. class term-root compound
4. class-prefixed root
It is also common to find quadrisyllabic compounds of the following types:
5. two-term compounds (in which “term” indicates one of types 1-4)
6. “expressive” compounds
Finally, a relatively small number of trisyllabic time nouns occur, with semantic values such as
“three days ago” or “four years from now”.
8
Mark Post
7. trisyllabic time nouns
We will next discuss and exemplify each of these seven types.
4.3.1. Prefixed roots
Prefixed roots consist of a single lexical root, plus one of three fairly abstract prefixes *ta-, *ja- or
*a-. The proto-functions of these prefixes are difficult to reconstruct, although differential
lexicalisations of prefixed roots in some languages suggest productivity at the Proto-Tani stage.
In the modern languages, ta- is found mainly on nouns denoting insects, lower animals and other
objects of the natural world (mostly, but not necessarily, small in size or diminutive in nature), as
well as physical properties relating to such natural objects. It also occurs as a “nicknaming”
prefix to the given name of a male child,8 when addressed by his parents or other elders, as well as
in other terms relating to males or to human beings more generally. Table 1 exemplifies the
distribution of ta-initial terms in Lare Galo; the first two columns give common nouns or
adjectives, while the third consists of terms relating to human males. Note that since prefixes are
not underlying Tone-Bearing Units (TBU) in Galo, the tone of a word consisting of a ta-prefixed
root is a direct projection of the underlying root tone.
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
tajúm
‘damp(ness)’
taín
‘mushroom’
taì
‘youngest son’
takèk
‘filth(y)’
taɨ ́
‘hail’
tapúu
‘whitey (of male)’
takám
‘clay’
taó
‘honeybee’
tapə́
‘nickname of a man named Kenpə’
takár
‘star’
tarùm
‘scorpion’
taníi
‘the father of humankind; the Tani
languages and their speakers’
Table 1. Representative set of terms with *ta- + root composition in Lare Galo
It may be possible to tentatively reconstruct a proto-value ‘Masculine (diminutive)’ for *ta-.
Similar to *ta-, although considerably less frequent, is *ja-, which is found on most colour
terms, certain diminutive objects, and certain concepts with an air of negativity. It is also found as
a nicknaming prefix to the given name of a girl, and some other terms relating to females. It may
be possible to reconstruct a proto-value ‘Feminine (diminutive)’ for *ja-. Table 2 exemplifies the
distribution of ja- initial terms in Lare Galo.
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
japúu
‘white’
jalùk
‘chili pepper’
japóm
‘fairy; demon’
jamàr
‘brown’
jalòo
‘slut; sexy’
jaì
‘youngest daughter’
jazì
‘yellow’
jarɨ ̀ɨ
‘starvation’
jatə̀r
‘nickname of a woman named Kentər’
Table 2. Representative set of terms with *ja- + root composition in Lare Galo
Proto-prefix *a- is perhaps even more abstract and difficult to trace. It occurs on a very large
number of nouns and adjectives denoting common and familiar, perhaps “basic” entities and their
8
In the Tani cultural-linguistic tradition, the disyllabic name of an individual is composed of a final syllable
relating to the individual him- or herself (the autosyllable) and an initial syllable derived from the autosyllable of the
individual’s father (the patrisyllable). Hence, Taníi has a son Niitóo, Niitóo has a son Toopó, and so on.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
9
properties, such as body part terms, kinship terms, numerals and classifiers in citation form,
common cultural artifacts, features of the natural world, physical properties and human
propensities. It is common to find modern reflexes of prefixed *a- changed via harmony with the
root nuclear vowel, although the set of affected terms is quite different from language to language,
apparently unpredictably so.9 Table 3 exemplifies a set of *a- initial terms in Lare Galo. Note
again that the prefix is not an underlying TBU, and the word tone directly reflects the tone of the
lexical root.
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
alák
‘arm including hand’
akò
‘old.INAN’
akèn
‘one; same’
alə̀
‘leg including foot’
ikìi
‘dog’
akòp
‘dent(ed)’
adín
‘meat’
udúu
‘bamboo section’
acì
‘(in) pain’
abó
‘father’
isì
‘water’
aɲɨ ̀ɨ
‘shy(ness)’
Table 3. Representative set of terms with *a- + root composition in Lare Galo
It seems clear that PT *a- is relatable to one or both of the prefixes a- and ʔa- found widely in
Tibeto-Burman languages of the greater Eastern Himalaya such as Lepcha, Meithei, Ao Naga,
Chin and Burmese, as well as elsewhere, as discussed in particular by Wolfenden (1929: 71-73)
and Lehman (1975), among others. However, unlike in some of these languages, in most modern
Tani languages its productivity is extremely limited; in most cases, it can only be described as the
initial vowel of an unanalysable lexeme.
However, one small but possibly telling area of the grammar in which a measure of
productivity is retained is in the classifier system. Unlike most nominal and adjectival roots,
which are not independently productive, classifier roots enter productively into construction with
numeral roots and a small handful of core adjectival roots to form disyllabic ‘classifier
expressions’ such as Lare Galo ikì dór-ɲì ‘dog CLF:4.LEG.ANIMAL-two’ ‘two dogs’ and ikì dór-tə̀
‘dog CLF:4.LEG.ANIMAL-big’ ‘big dog’. The citation form of a classifier, which may be used
either alone with individual reference or in construction with a higher numeral, takes a prefix a-,
as in adór ‘a four-legged animal’ or abór ‘a spread-out thing’ (< bór- ‘CLF:SPREAD’). Unlike
common nouns, the a-prefix of a classifier never appears to undergo harmony with the root
syllable nucleus, which presumably reflects its active, productive status. As the previous
translations suggest, the resulting word denotes an individual of a given semantic type, rather
than, e.g. the semantic type itself (construed as an abstract concept).10 It may be that the protofunction of *a- as a prefix to common nominal roots had a similar sort of individualising function.
As for *a-prefixed adjectives, it is possible that they developed out of a similar type of
concretisation operation which derived adjectives from verbs. A number of verb/adjective pairs
exist in modern Tani languages which serve to support such a notion, such as Lare Galo adɨ ́k
‘irritation/irritating’ and dɨ ́k- ‘irritate’, and arék ‘sharp (of the edge of a blade)’ and rék- ‘be sharp
(of a blade)’; however, the derivation, if that is what is was, no longer appears to be productive.
9 Cf. Lare Galo isì versus Paglo Mising asi ‘water’, or Apatani òhò ‘child’ (Abraham 1987: 21) versus Lare Galo
aò and Bokar aho (Sun 1993: 109).
10 An alternative formation in which the classifier root is compounded to the numeral root kèn- ‘one’ as in
dorkèn or borkèn have almost the same semantic value as adór or abór, although the former are somewhat preferred
when enumerating, or when the individual status of a referent is being profiled and/or contrasted with a quantity.
10
Mark Post
It is important here to underline the fact that in themselves, *a-prefixed adjectives do not
denote individuals with a given property, but rather denote the property itself. Thus, if indeed an
‘individualising’ function such as that found on Tani classifiers, as well as among deverbal or
deadjectival nouns in Southern Chin (Lehman 1975: 29), can be reconstructed to Proto-Tani or a
near ancestor thereof, its dissolution in most areas of the nominal/adjectival lexicon seems to have
occurred soon thereafter.
4.3.2. Root-root compounds
Root-root compounds are found with great frequency among nouns and adjectives alike. The
usual pattern underlying their formation is a classificatory Generic-Specific one, in which the first
term identifies a semantic type of entity or condition, and the second a kind or manner of the type
(Table 4). Thus, dùm- in dumpìn invokes a Generic class of entities of the semantic type ‘deer’
(or ‘things to do with deer’), and pìn- ‘skin’ then narrows the reference. Similarly, bée- in beehòr
invokes the type ‘things to do with monkeys’ while hòr- ‘length/long’ fixes the kind. Although
this sort of patterning shares much in common with the “taxonomic” compound patterning
commonly found in the languages of Southeast Asia, it is important to note that the Tani pattern is
not reducible to an “order of modification” MOD-H (or H-MOD) as many patterns described as
taxonomic appear to be. Although the “order of modification” in terms like dumpìn and beehòr
would appear mixed (since dumpìn is a ‘kind’ of skin, but beehòr is a ‘kind’ of monkey), the fact
is that the concepts of headship and modification simply do not apply to the composition of Tani
compounds. For further discussion, see Post (in progress).
In Galo, the tone of a root-root compound is derived from the underlying tones of its
constituent roots, according to the following patterns: HH → H, HL → L, LH → L, LL → L (with
an infrequently-occurring Rising-Falling tone also patterning like Low). The resulting High,
Low, or (more rarely) Rising-Falling tone is realised as a surface contour over the domain of the
phonological word.
Compound
Root 1
Root 2
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
dumpìn
‘deerskin’
dùm-
‘deer’
pìn-
‘skin’
beehòr
‘langur’
bée-
‘monkey’
hòr-
‘length/long’
dumpúu
‘white hair(ed)’
dúm-
‘head (hair)’
púu-
‘white’
hibùu
‘river’
hì-
‘water’
búu-
‘pipe’
Table 4. Composition of selected Lare Galo nominal/adjectival root-root compounds
4.3.3. Class term-root compounds
“Class term” is used here in the sense of DeLancey (1986, citing Haas) with reference to the Thai
system of nominal classification. In Thai as in Tani, class terms act as the Generic element in a
Generic-Specific compound template. They are distinguished from ordinary Generic compound
elements in Thai for three reasons: 1) frequency 2) productivity and 3) frequent ability to function
as Classifiers. In (10), the class term ráan – which, like most Thai class terms, has the ability to
stand as a free noun with the same semantic value – is “repeated” as the classifier for the
compound ráan-ʔaahǎan.11
11
For related discussion, see Hundius and Kölver (1983) and Post (in press-a).
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
11
(10) T ráan-ʔaahǎan sǎam ráan
shop-food
three CLF:SHOP
‘three restaurants’ (lit. ~ ‘three shops of restaurant’)
In Tani, class terms are frequent, and function as Generic elements to a usually large number
of compounds. Their productivity in modern Tani languages is limited, in the sense that the
compounds in which they occur are not in general actively formed; however, based on frequency
of occurrence in the modern languages we can perhaps assume a formerly high productivity.
Table 5 illustrates a lexical set organised around the class term lɨ ̀ɨ- ‘stone’. In the interest of
space, the individual roots are not glossed. Note also that class terms are like (other) compounded
roots, and unlike prefixes, in being underlying TBUs.
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
lɨɨcàk
‘pebble’
lɨɨpùu
‘marble’
lɨɨkə̀r
‘turquoise’
lɨɨmɨ ̀k
‘gravel’
lɨɨnə̀
‘boulder’
lɨɨcùm
‘jade’
lɨɨmìk
‘algae’
lɨɨpùm
‘stone pile’
lɨɨpə̀
‘sharpening stone’
lɨɨtàk
‘giant boulder’
lɨɨtòr
‘hard stone’
lɨɨkə̀
‘onyx’
lɨɨkàa
‘igneous rock’
lɨɨjàa
‘soft stone’
lɨɨcɨ ̀k
‘cooking tripod’
Table 5. Lare Galo Lexical set based on the class term lɨ ̀ɨ-‘stone’
Tani languages vary in the extent to which class terms feed into their classifier systems. In the
Galo example (11), the root pɨ ̀- ‘egg; globe’ occurs as both Generic and Specific elements of the
compound noun ‘egg’ (final -ɨ ̀ has undergone a regular process of Word-final vowel weakening
postdating the Proto-Galo stage). The same etymological root then classifies the noun; note that
this is essentially the same pattern as in the Thai example (10). However, (12) shows that the
pattern is not productive at least in Galo; nor is it productive in Mising. However, there are
indications that in Apatani, the classifier system can be expanded in precisely this way (Abraham
1985: 65-66); further research must establish the extent to which the Apatani construction is truly
productive.
(11) G pɨpə̀
pɨpə̀
pɨúmgo
pɨ-úm=go
egg
CLF:GLOBE-three=IND
‘three eggs’
(12) G lɨɨkə̀
lɨɨkə̀
pɨúmgo
pɨ-úm=go
onyx CLF:GLOBE-three=IND
‘three pieces of onyx’
*lɨɨkə̀ lɨúmgo
12
Mark Post
4.3.4. Class prefix-root compounds
A relatively small number of compound initials appear likely to have at one time occurred as class
terms, but are not easily reconstructible to a lexical root source. They are best considered
prefixes, and like the prefixes discussed in §4.3.1 they are not underlying TBUs; also like
prefixes, they are sometimes found to have been subject to vowel harmonisation with the root
syllable nucleus. Nevertheless, the common semantics of the terms in which they appear are in
most cases transparent, and in this respect they more closely resemble class terms. Examples
from Lare Galo are ho- ‘4-legged animal prefix’ (< PT *sV-, prob. < PTB *sya ‘flesh’ (Matisoff
2003: 651)), pə- ‘bird prefix’ (< PT *pV-, prob. < PTB *bya ‘bird’ (Matisoff 2003: 641)), doo‘heaven(ly) prefix’ (< PT *doŋ-) and kVV- ‘flavour prefix’ (poss. < PTB *ka ‘mouth’ (Matisoff
2003: 659)). Table 6 exemplifies a set from Lare Galo.
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
Term
Gloss
hobìn
‘goat; takin’
pəzàp
‘duck’
doomùk
‘vapour’
hocə́r
‘deer’
pəgáa
‘great pied hornbill’
kaacàk
‘bitter’
hozòo
‘flying squirrel’
dooɲí
‘sun’
kuucùk
‘sour’
pətáa
‘bird’
doogúm
‘storm; thunder’
keebèk
‘starchy’
Table 6. Class-prefixed roots in Lare Galo
4.3.5. “Larger” compounds
In this section, we briefly discuss two-term compounds, “expressive” compounds and trisyllabic
compounds.
Two-term compounds are essentially compounds of compounds; i.e. they contain two terms
from one of the four types discussed above. Two-term compounds may be asymmetrical,
following the Generic-Specific patterning also found in root-root compounds, or symmetrical, in
which both terms are Specific, and relate to a Generic concept which is the denotation of the
whole. Such structures are also sometimes described as “coordinative” compounds.
“Expressive” compounds formally resemble two-term compounds. However, while the first
term is generally analysable, the second term consists of a semi-reduplication of the first term, and
has no independent semantic value. These may be compared to the “elaborate expressions” found
in many morphosyllabic languages (Matisoff 1988: 39).
Finally, trisyllabic compounds are found among time nouns with a complex reference such as
“three days ago” or “four years hence”.12 The compositionality of many of these is difficult to
completely analyse at present.
Table 7 exemplifies these three “larger” compound types in Lare Galo.
12 Time nouns in Tani operate on a base eight system, in which the “present” is a shifting deictic center, and up to
seven units (days, nights, or years) are counted forwards or backwards thence.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
13
Two-term compounds
Expressive compounds
Time nouns
Term
îs-hilə̀ə
Term
abûk-arùk
Term
keŋkəɲɨ ̀ɨ
Gloss
‘three years ago’
kendaɲɨ ̀ɨ
‘four years ago’
kendalòo
‘four days ago’
Gloss
‘lake’ (‘water’-‘deep
section of river’)
‘mature man’
(‘elder bro.’-‘father’)
‘wild animal’
(‘bear’-‘boar’)
ác-abó
hottúmhorə́
acên-arèn
akîn-amìn
Gloss
‘skin flareup’
(‘pod’-‘x’)
‘be decided’
(‘believe’-‘x’)
‘muddled’
(‘messy’-‘x’)
Table 7. “Larger” compounds in Lare Galo (“x” indicates “no independent meaning”)
4.3.6. Compounds, prefixed roots and the cline of semantic generality
It is possible to analyse Tani compounds and prefixed roots in terms of a cline of semantic
generality, from more particular on the one side, to more general on the other. On the first side
are two-term compounds, which are high in semantic particularity, unlikely to have a large
number of exemplars, and may represent an actively-formed collocation. On the other side are
prefixed roots, in which a highly abstract prefix with earlier functionality and a consequently large
number of exemplars is now frozen in lexical representations together with any sound changes
that pertained to it. In the middle are terms at intermediate stages of loss of semantic
particularities and productivity, and phonological integration.
two-term compound
root-root compound
Semantically particular
Small number of exemplars
Tone derives from two words
Rule-governed internal sandhi
Productive/actively formed
class term-root compound
prefixed root
Semantically general
Large number of exemplars
Tone derives from two roots
Tone derives from one root
Unpredictably variable internal sandhi
Unproductive/lexicalised
Figure 3. Cline of semantic generality and productivity in nominal/adjectival compounds and prefixed
roots
4.3.7. Zero derivation among nouns and adjectives
Perhaps due to their parallel evolution and strong formal resemblance, zero-derivation among
nouns and adjectives in Tani languages such as Galo and Mising is straightforward. Often, it is all
but impossible to determine whether the nominal or the adjectival form is the more “basic” (1314).
(13) G êe!
ee
adɨggó!
adɨ ́k=go
irritation=IND
‘Wah! What a bother!’
DISM
(14) G hɨgɨ ̀
hɨgɨ ̀
PRX.IND
maazîb
adɨgdù.
maazî=bə́
adɨ ́k-dùu
very=AVZR irritating-IPFV
‘This is really burning (of a wound).’
14
Mark Post
4.4. Evolution of the verbal lexicon
Verbs in Tani underwent a quite different set of developments to those of Tani nouns and
adjectives. Although, in general, a similar shift from basic monosyllabism to basic polysyllabism
appears to have occurred, the trend was not toward compounding of verb roots according to a
Generic-Specific pattern (or any other sort of pattern). No verbal prefixes whatsoever appear to
have been attested in any Tani language to date, and few if any compound verbs are found.
Instead, an asymmetry developed between verb roots on the one hand and a wide variety of
verbal suffixes on the other. Although a full exposition of the set of verbal suffixes available
even for a single Tani language well exceeds the scope of this paper, it is possible to draw a
preliminary division between suffixes with derivational and inflectional functions.
Inflectional suffixes may occur distant from the root, and code functions related to aspect,
modality, and finiteness. We will not be concerned with these further in the present paper.
Derivational suffixes, with which we will be concerned here, are common and plentiful in all
adequately-described Tani languages. Occurring directly adjacent to a verb root and forming a
usually disyllabic word with it, derivational verb suffixes may be easily mistaken for compound
elements; however, whereas root-root compounding is generally unproductive in modern Tani
languages, derivational verb suffixes are always highly productive, and occur on any semantically
compatible root. Furthermore, although many derivational verb suffixes are semantically rich,
many have no modern cognate verb root. Although it is possible – and indeed, likely – that much
variation is found among the derivational verb suffixes of various Tani languages, the system
itself as it is described here appears to be shared among Mising, Galo, and Apatani, representing
Eastern, transitional Western, and Western Tani respectively, and can therefore perhaps be safely
set forth as a prototypically Tani system.
4.4.1. Derivational verb suffixes – functions
As noted above, derivational verb suffixes code a wide array of functions in most Tani languages.
Most of these comfortably fall under one of the following types:
1) class change (nominalisation, adjectivalisation)
2) valence change (redistribution, increase)
3) adverbial modification (manner, result, direction)
4) aspectual modification
We will treat these types in order, again using data from Lare Galo to illustrate.
4.4.1.1. Class change derivations
A large number of suffixes perform nominalisations, and an even larger number are
adjectivalisers. All are semantically rich, and may have other functions in addition to class
change. Example (15) illustrates use of the ‘reason (for)’ nominaliser -dín, while (16) exemplifies
the adjectivaliser -kèn ‘good/easy (to)’; recall from (5) that copular predication is possible for
adjectives but not verb roots; without an adjectivaliser such as -kèn, (16) would be
ungrammatical.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
(15) G nók
nó-kə̀
əmbə̀
mendinə̀
jôoə
la?
əmbə̀
mèn-dín=əə
jôo=əə
la
2.SG-GEN
like.that
speak-NZR:REAS=TOP what=COP.IPFV
‘What’s the reason for your talking like that?’
(16) G aré!
aré
15
CQ
dokènə!
dó-kèn=əə
eat-AZR:GOOD=COP.IPFV
‘Wow! It’s actually tasty (I expected it would not be)!’
SURP
4.4.1.2. Valence change derivations
Valence changing suffixes may increase or decrease the number of arguments required or
supported by a verb root, or reassign the semantic roles which would ordinarily obtain to them. In
(17), the ‘insert’ Applicative -lɨ ̀k adds a goal argument to the frame of the transitive verb mín‘chase’, understood as a location into which the O argument referent is manipulated.13 In (18),
the Argument Reversal suffix -kò causes the default semantic role assignment of tùp- ‘headbutt’
(A → Agent, O → Patient) to be reversed; note that this is not in fact a passive, since the syntax is
not in any way affected.
(17) G howwə́m
minlktok!
hoə́=əm
mín-lk-tó-k
cattle=ACC
chase-APPL:INSERT-IPTV.ODIR-ADM
‘Drive the cows into there!’
(18) G mərò,
bədaaló
nennəmə̀,
məròo
bədáa=lo
nèn-nam=əə
yesterday
road=LOC
exit-NZR.PF=TOP
bɨ ̀
gariəm
tupkòto
bɨ ̀ɨ
gari=əm
túp-kò-tó
3.SG vehicle(<Asm)=ACC head.butt-REV.ARG-PFV
‘Yesterday, he was just stepping out onto the road (when) he was hit by a car.’
4.4.1.3. Adverbial modification
Adverbial modification of a predicate may include providing information concerning the manner
of the predicated event or state (e.g. ‘playfully’, as in 19), concerning some direction to a
predicated action (e.g. ‘outward’, also 19) or concerning some outcome or result (20).
13 Although the goal argument is covert, its underlying existence is clearly inferred; without -lɨ̀k, the sense of (17)
would be ‘catch up with the cows!’
16
Mark Post
(19) G aló
gollo,
kobù
dorúmgo…
alóo
go=lo
kobùu dór-úm=go
day
IND=LOC
rodent CLF:4.LEG.ANIMAL-three=IND
oodôobə
immên tabə̀
inlênto.
oodòo=bə́
ín-mèn-tà=bə́
ín-lèn-tó
far=AVZR
walk-AS.PLAY-INCP=SBRD go-OUT-PFV
‘One day, three mice went far away out to take a walk.’
(20) G jôojoəm
jôo-jôo=əm
rɨ ̀dû,
ŋun
cencî raakumá.
rɨ ̀-duù
ŋunù
cèn-cì-ráa-kú-máa
what-what=ACC
do-IPFV
1.PL know-REACH-ODD.ONE.OUT-CMPL-NEG
‘What sort of stuff (you) are doing, we (old folks) don’t really understand it anymore.’
4.4.1.4. Aspectual modification
Inflectional suffixes code the majority of aspectual functions on finite verbs. However,
derivational suffixes perform a number of aspectual modifications which are unrelated to verb
finiteness; Durative, Tentative, Persistive, Iterative, Incipient, and Habitual are among the various
aspectual functions derivational suffixes may code. An example of Incipient suffix use in a
subordinated verb is found in (19). Example (21) illustrates use of the Durative suffix -bə́ə (< PG
*bə́ə ‘carry; hold; bear’) and the Tentative suffix -káa (< PTS *kaŋ ‘look’).
hocə́r abnámgo...
tuulɨ ̂gnamgo
nó-kə̀
hocə́r áp-nam=go
tùu-lɨ ̀k-nam=go
2.SG-GEN
deer
push.with.force-APPL:INSERT-NZR=IND
membə̂ədu,
əgə̀m
(21) G nòk...
shoot-NZR=IND
mèn-bə́ə-dùu əgə̀=əm
ɨɨzikáato.
ɨ ́ɨ-zí-káa-tó
say-DUR-IPFV DST.IND=ACC narrate-BEN-TENT-IPTV.ODIR
‘You’re always talking about your…stag-shooting incident, when you shoved it into
(somewhere), go ahead and tell him about that.’
4.4.2. Historical source of derivational verb suffixes
Of the 100-plus derivational verb suffixes attested by Post (in progress) for Galo and by Lorrain
(1995 [1910]) for Mising, between 40-60% can be easily traced to lexical roots (almost always
verb roots) with the same segmental phonology and underlying tone. For example, the Galo
Benefactive suffix -zí exemplified in (21) has a verb root counterpart zí- ‘give’. Also, as
discussed in §3.2, even when a source lexeme has obsolesced in a given language, it is often
possible to find it in another Tani language, such as Mising rɨk- ‘meet’ & Galo -rɨ ́k ‘Applicative’.
Derivational verb suffixes frequently retain many of the semantic characteristics of their lexical
source forms, but deploy these productively in the more generalised service of grammatical
functions.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
17
It is interesting to note, then, that the serial verbs common to the morphosyllabic languages of
Mainland Southeast Asia do this too (Matisoff 1969). Indeed, many of the functions of Tani
derivational suffixes recall those of post-head serial verbs in languages like Lahu, Thai and
Chinese. Table 8 compares some Galo derivational suffixes with some functionally analogous
post-head serial verbs in Thai.
Function
Galo
Thai
Gloss
Manner
ín-mèn
dó-ŋám
gá-càa
mèn-zí
ín-bə́ə
dəən lên
kin (hâj) mòt
piin khɨ ̂n
bɔ̀ɔk hâj
dəən jùu
‘stroll (walk as play)’
Result
Direction
Valence +
Aspect
‘eat all up’
‘climb up’
‘tell to/for someone’
‘(be) walk(ing) continuously’
Table 8. Comparison of Galo derivational verb suffixes and Thai post-head serial verbs
In addition, derivational verb suffixes may be found split across iterations of a verb root.
Together with the “expressive” compounds discussed in §4.3.5, these constructions closely recall
the four-syllable “elaborate expressions” of Mainland Southeast Asia; compare the second clause
predicate in (22) with one of many similar Lahu expressions, such as šɛ-phûʔ-šɛ-ku ‘writheturn.over-writhe-RDUP’ ‘twist and turn’ (Matisoff 1988: 1211).
(22) G botolə
botol=əə
takkáaku…
taŋmɨ ̂k
taŋmâk káaku!
ták-káa-kú
ták-mɨ ̀k
ták-màk-káa-kú
bottle(<Eng)=TOP
crack-PF-CMPL
crack-INTO.DUST
‘The bottle broke…smashed into a million pieces!’
crack-RDUP-PF-CMPL
There is every possibility that the pan-Tani system of derivational verb suffixes originated
historically in verb serialisation.
5. CONCLUSION
In this paper, I have tried to do two things: first, I have tried to show that the Tani languages
appear likely to have had an essentially morphosyllabic past, but that they have, in general, shifted
toward greater synthesis and agglutination (albeit in different ways and to different degrees).
Next, I tried to outline how this change appears to have structured the lexicon such that while
nouns, adjectives and verbs are synchronically represented as three distinct classes (at least in
some modern Tani languages), nouns and adjectives share some common structural and
behavioural characteristics which are not shared by verbs.
This is potentially an interesting alignment, because we are perhaps more used to adjectives in
the languages of almost all of Asia being more closely aligned to verbs. However, it’s also
potentially explainable. Many of the languages for which adjectives are claimed to occur as a
subclass of verbs are morphosyllabic languages; as such, they tend to lack well-grammaticalised
material that could serve to grammatically mark what are after all semantically-based classdistinctions (Dixon 2004). The progressive lexicalisation of prefixed expressions may have in
part enabled structural expression of an adjective-verb opposition which is already incipient in
many morphosyllabic languages (see e.g. Post (in press-b) for a description of adjectives in Thai),
18
Mark Post
and the result is a fairly stratified lexicon. In some modern Tani languages, adjectives may be
moving closer to verbal status again, particularly in those languages which lack a copula which is
not homophonous with the pan-Tani Imperfective suffix -duuŋ (< PTS *duŋ ‘sit’). But that’s
another story.
ABBREVIATIONS
1
3
Asm
Eng
First person
Third person
Assamese
Abilitative
Accusative
Admonitive
Applicative
Adverbialiser
Adjectivaliser
Benefactive
Classifier
Completive
Copula
Content question
Dismissive
Distal demonstrative
Durative
English
EXT
GEN
H
HORT
IMMD
INAN
Extensive
Genitive
High tone
Hortative
Immediate
Inanimate
ABIL
ACC
ADM
APPL
AVZR
AZR
BEN
CLF
CMPL
COP
CQ
DISM
DST
DUR
INCP
IND
IPFV
IPTV
IRR
L
LOC
NEG
NF
NZR
ODIR
PF
PFV
PL
PQ
PRX
REAS
RDUP
REV.ARG
SBRD
SFOC
SG
SURP
TENT
TOP
Incipient
Individuative
Imperfective
Imperative
Irrealis
Low tone
Locative
Negative
Non-finite
Nominaliser
Outward (non-self) directed
Perfect
Perfective
Plural
Polar question
Proximate demonstrative
Reason
Reduplication
Reverse core argument roles
Subordinator
Sequential focus
Singular
Surprise
Tentative
Topic
REFERENCES
ABRAHAM, P. T. 1985. Apatani Grammar. Mysore: Central Institute of Indian Languages.
––––––. 1987. Apatani-English-Hindi Dictionary. Mysore: Central Institute of Indian
Languages.
BLACKBURN, Stuart. 2003/2004. “Memories of migration: Notes on legends and beads in
Arunachal Pradesh, India.” European Bulletin of Himalayan Research 25/26,15-60.
DELANCEY, Scott. 1986. “Toward a History of Tai Classifier Systems.” Noun Classes and
Categorization, Colette CRAIG (ed.), 437-452.
Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John
Benjamins.
DIXON, R. M. W. 2004. “Adjective classes in typological perspective.” Adjective Classes: A
Cross-Linguistic Typology, R.M.W. DIXON and Alexandra AIKHENVALD (eds.), 1-49.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
DONEGAN, Patricia J. & STAMPE, David. 1983. “Rhythm and the holistic organization of
language structure.” J.F. Richardson et al. (eds.) Papers from the Parasession on the
Interplay of Phonology, Morphology and Syntax 19.2, 337-353. Chicago: CLS.
––––––. 2004. “Rhythm and the synthetic drift of Munda.” R. Singh (ed.) The Yearbook of
South Asian Languages and Linguistics, 3-36. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
HUNDIUS, Harald and Ulrike KÖLVER. 1983. “Syntax and semantics of numeral classifiers in
Thai.” Studies in Language 7, 165-214.
Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon
19
LEHMAN, F. K. 1975. “Wolfenden’s non-pronominal a-prefix in Tibeto-Burman: Two
arguments from Southern Chin and some proposed semantic correlates.” LTBA 2.1, 19-44.
LIGHT, Timothy 1978. “Tonogenesis: Analysis and Implications.” Lingua 46.2-3, 115-131.
LORRAIN, J. Herbert 1995 [1910]. A Dictionary of the Abor-Miri Language, with Illustrative
Sentences and Notes. New Delhi: Mittal.
MATISOFF, James A. 1969. “Verb concatenation in Lahu: The syntax and semantics of ‘simple’
juxtaposition.” Acta Linguistica Hafniensiae 12, 69-120.
––––––. 1988. The Dictionary of Lahu. Berkeley: University of California Press.
––––––. 2003. Handbook of Proto-Tibeto-Burman. Berkeley: University of California Press.
OUYANG, Jueya. 1985. Luobazu Yuyan Jianzhi: Bengni Boga'eryu (Brief Description of the
Luoba Nationality Language: the Bengni-Bogar Language). Beijing: Nationality Press.
POST, Mark. 2005. “Synchronic and diachronic dimensions of the tone system of Galo.” Paper
presented at the 36th Annual Meeting of the Australian Linguistics Society, Monash
University, Melbourne, September 28-30, 2005.
––––––. in press-a. “Grammaticalisation and compounding in Thai and Chinese.” Studies in
Language.
––––––. in press-b. “Adjectives in Thai.” Proceedings from the 13th Annual Meeting of the
Southeast Asian Linguistics Society, University of California at Los Angeles, May 2003.
––––––. in progress. A Grammar of Galo. Melbourne: La Trobe University dissertation.
SUN, Jackson T.-S. 1993. A Historical-Comparative Study of the Tani (Mirish) Branch of
Tibeto-Burman. Berkeley: University of California dissertation.
––––––. 2003. “Tani languages.” The Sino-Tibetan Languages. Graham THURGOOD and Randy
J. LAPOLLA (eds.), 456-466. London, New York: Routledge.
WOLFENDEN, Stuart N. 1929. Outlines of T-B Linguistic Morphology. London: Royal Asiatic
Society.